These are excerpts and elaborations from my book "The Nature of Consciousness"
Preface By the time you finish reading this book you will be a different
person. I am not claiming that this
book will change the way you think and act. I am simply referring to the fact
that the cells in your body, including the neurons of your brain, are continuously
changing. By the time you finish reading this book you will
"literally" be a different body and a different brain. Every word
that you read is having an effect on the connections between your neurons. And
every breath you take is pacing the metabolism of your cells. This book is
about what just happened to you. As with any book worth
reading, the goal of this book is to fill a gap. In my case, the gap is a lack
of books that provide an interdisciplinary account of the studies on the mind
carried out around the world. While many books carry that label, most of them
focus on the one or two disciplines or theories that the author intends to
defend or attack. First and foremost, my book
aims at providing an accessible and stimulating introduction to those studies
across a number of disciplines: Philosophy, Psychology, Computer Science,
Mathematics, Biology, Neurology and Physics. This book contains a brief
description of every single modern theory (about Consciousness, Cognition and
Life) of which I am aware. This book was originally
born to provide an overview for ordinary humans of the philosophical mind-body
debate, of neurological models of the brain, of computational theories of
cognition (Artificial Intelligence, Connectionism), of post-Darwinian biology,
of theories on memory, reasoning, learning, emotions, common sense, dreams,
language, metaphor, of modern Physics (Quantum Theory, Non-equilibrium
Thermodynamics, Relativity Theory), and, last but not least, consciousness. It
was originally meant as a compendium of the scientific ideas that are likely to
shape the intellectual scenario of the third millennium. And I was its original
reader. This book also offers a
humble personal opinion on what the solution to the mystery of consciousness
may be. But that is not the centerpiece of the book. A popular question of our
times is: What is the meaning of life? I always found that question misleading,
because first we should be able to answer the more basic question: What is the
meaning of matter? This book can’t answer either, but at least tries to make
the connection. Physics has explained
everything we have found in the universe. We know how the universe started and
how it will end. We know what drives it. We know what makes it. Our knowledge
of fundamental forces and elementary particles is increasing daily. Two things
remain to be explained: how am I alive and how do I think. What does it take
for something to be alive and to think? Can we "build" a machine that
thinks and is alive? What is thought (consciousness)? And what is life? Physics
provides no answer. Historically speaking, Physics never tried to give an
answer. Life and thought were considered beyond the reach of formulas. Today,
instead, scientists from different disciplines view living and thinking as
physical phenomena to be studied the same way we study galaxies and
electricity. The most important revolution of our century could be the idea
that thinking and living can be explained by mathematical formulas, just like
any other phenomenon in the universe. Science may never be the same again,
literally. Any scientific theory that does not provide a credible account for
consciousness and life is faulted from the beginning, as it ignores the two
phenomena its own existence depends upon. We are alive and we are conscious: we
know that much. We live in an age in which
the study of consciousness, cognition and life is no longer philosophical
speculation. It is, instead, affecting a growing number of disciplines. For the
first time in history there is a convergence of specialists (neurologists,
biologists, physicists, mathematicians, computer scientists, psychologists)
onto one subject. A new view of nature is
slowly emerging, which encompasses both galaxies and neurons, gravitation and
life, molecules and emotions. In what represents the culmination of
centuries of studying nature, humankind is now approaching the thorniest subject
of all: ourselves. We are part
of nature, but, historically, Science left us in the background, limiting our
role to the one of observers. For a long time we humans
have enjoyed this privileged status. But we seem no longer capable of eluding
the fundamental issue: that what we have been studying for all these centuries
is but us, albeit disguised under theories of the universe and of elementary
particles (theories of what “we” see). And now it is about time that we focus
on the real subject. The human mind appears to us as the ultimate and most refined product of life. And life
appears to us as the ultimate and most refined product
of matter. Now we need a theory
of the universe in which consciousness, cognition and life are not oddities,
but building blocks. The fact that we do not have
yet a good theory of mind probably
means that we do not have a good theory
of the universe. Consciousness is perhaps the great mystery of the universe.
And the reason may very well lie in a fundamental inadequacy of our Science to
explain natural phenomena. In a sense, the new science of mind is doing more
than just studying mind: it is indirectly reformulating the program of Science
in general. Future generations will be
amazed that it took thousands of years (and hundreds of years since the
scientific revolution) to realize how important consciousness is to understand
the world and ourselves. At every point in the
history of Science, a paradigm shift allowed for the explanation of previously
unexplained phenomena. The challenge, now, is to
explain why we are here. And what we are. Ultimately, this book is
about the gap between "I" and "me". This book was begun in 1997.
It was published as “Thinking about Thought” in 2003. It has been greatly
revised and expanded since then. This new edition includes material that
originally appeared on my website www.scaruffi.com.
Click on “Science” and register to the monthly newsletter if you wish to
receive future updates. The website is also the
easiest way to find out my email address. I welcome feedback from readers,
whether it is typos or opinions. Piero Scaruffi Redwood City, November 2006 Few people have the qualifications to write such an ambitious survey of
a brand new discipline. I confess i am not one of them. After leading the
Olivetti Artificial Intelligence Center for several years, in 1995-96 i spent
two years at Stanford University studying Cognitive Science (thanks to a
visiting scholarship kindly granted by Robert Engelmore at the Knowledge
Systems Laboratory). That was, in retrospect, the beginning of this book. I
simply gathered information from all disciplines with the aim of working out a
synthesis of sorts. I guess it will remain the goal of my life, although i have
missed my chance of working inside academia. My background is a mess. I graduated in Mathematics but my thesis was on
theoretical Physics (that’s where i got my introduction to Quantum and
Relativity Physics). I worked in the software industry and eventually did
research on Artificial Intelligence. In my other lives, i write about music, cinema and literature, and i am
working on a history of knowledge from the beginning of human civilization to
our days. I have published poetry that won a few awards. I have traveled to
more than 120 countries. I think that all readers will be interested in the main ideas surveyed within each chapter, but probably most readers will not be interested in the details of each and every chapter. Each chapter contains a
short introduction to the subject, and then a series of paragraphs that
summarize the theories of the main specialists in the field. I rarely take sides. I
summarize a scholar's work and let you decide. (I apologize with the scholars: a one-page summary of their work is, of course, a very superficial reading of their theories. The goal of this book is to offer “breadth”, not “depth”). What is subjective is the selection: i do make a selection for the reader, not only focusing on theories that represent real paradigm shifts but also omitting (with a few exceptions) those theories that are too unscientific for my taste. Much of what i read in Psychology and Philosophy, for example, is incredibly trivial or incompetent (unaware of research in other fields). As you advance into the
chapter, the theories get more difficult and sometimes repetitive. Depending on
your level of interest, you may want to absorb all the details or just skip to
the next chapter. My own ideas are usually
left for the end of each chapter. Needless to say, you don't miss much if you
skip my ideas. I have a feeling that, for most readers, the best way to read this book is in many stages: first surf the chapters (focusing on the first half of each chapter), then re-read the book going a bit further within each chapter. A generous bibliography at the end of each chapter should help you
select what you want to read next, depending on what intrigued you most (titles
in bold are those recommended for beginners). The core material on Cognitive Science is divided into chapters that
roughly correspond to cognitive faculties (memory, dreams, emotions, language,
etc). But the “peripheral” chapters are no less important, and in fact, take up
most of the book. There is a first chapter devoted to a survey of Philosophy of
Mind, followed by three chapters on Machine Intelligence. There is a lengthy
chapter on Physics flanked by three chapters on Biology. Beginning with a survey of Philosophy may not be the best way to
introduce a book that promotes a new science. While Philosophy matters less
when hard data are available, philosophers of mind did frame the problem. The
reader probably has her own strong opinion on what consciousness is and where
it comes from. After reading the first chapter and the different theories of
all those philosophers, the reader with strong opinions will probably realize
that her convictions are a bit amateurish. Thus that first chapter may be
helpful to “clear the air”. Ditto for Machine Intelligence. Whether “intelligent” (or, better,
conscious) machines are feasible or not, the program of building one has forced
people to think about what consciousness is and where it comes from. The fact
that we are not even close to building such a machine means that it is either
impossible or we are still in the dark. The chapters on Biology provide the kind of general knowledge that is
needed to situate cognition in the proper context. Our brain acts in a world.
An organism does not live in an empty universe. The origin of life and its
evolution are also relevant. The nature of altruism (that seems to defy the
essence of Darwinism) is important to understand our mind. The physical and
mathematical theories of life are basically abstracting general principles
applicable to other fields (possibly neurobiology itself). Throughout these
chapters i introduce the basic facts of life (e.g., the genetic code, DNA, the
structure of the cell, etc). The chapter on the brain is relatively straightforward: it summarizes
what we know about the structure and functioning of the brain. Ditto for the
three chapters on Biology. Both Neurology and Biology are essential to
understand the “what” and the “why” of cognition. The chapters on dreams and
emotions are much more speculative. There are four chapters on Language, each one addressing a different
dimension (structure, origin, metaphor, pragmatics). The chapter on meaning is
rather technical (by the standards of this book) but i felt that a brief
introduction to the debate on truth and meaning could not be omitted. Skip it
if it sounds tedious. The chapter on Physics is essential. Every other chapter is useful for
its philosophical, biological, mathematical or psychological speculations, but,
at the end of the day, it is Physics that best summarizes our knowledge of the
universe. Anybody who has an opinion on life, cognition or consciousness
without being fluent in Quantum Mechanics and Relativity is simply bluffing. The last chapters attack consciousness, surveying all the main theories
that I am aware of. The three chapters cover biological theories of
consciousness (that are based on classical Physics), theories of how
consciousness emerged in evolution, and finally theories of consciousness based
on modern Physics (not classical Physics). Like in most of the previous
chapters, do not be surprised if you find contradictions: there is hardly any
consensus yet on any of these theories. Physics is amazingly monolithic in its
beliefs of what matter is and how it works. No such certainties exist in the
science of consciousness. Finally, there is a separate chapter on theories of the
self, although that has been discussed in previous chapters on the brain and on
consciousness. The last chapter is a collection of answers to “frequently asked
questions”. As i lecture on these topics, the audience invariably ask me the
same questions. For many years i resisted giving any credibility to these
questions, but finally accepted the fact that i cannot elude them forever. The character of these chapters varies wildly. Depending on your background and interests, the first three or four
chapters may excite you or put you to sleep. Whichever it turns out to be, do
not assume that the rest of the chapters are of the same kind. In a sense, each
one reflects the style and method of the discipline that it summarizes. Previous readers consistently praised the second half of the book and
criticized the first half. I still think that the first (more technical) half
is necessary to understand the context of the second (more speculative) half.
Books that only deal with life or consciousness are not educating the reader:
they are merely teasing the reader. Back to the index of all chapters |
![]() | ![]() | ![]() | ![]() |